“IUSSI Meets in St. Petersburg, Russia: A Feast of Firsts, Parts I-III”
American Bee Journal (November
2005-January 2006), Vols. 913-914
By
Dr. Malcolm T.
Sanford
http://apis.shorturl.com
St. Petersburg
is the Russian Federation’s
second largest and most European-like city with a population exceeding 4.5
million, fourth in size in Europe after Moscow, London and Paris. Constructed on the low-lying delta of the Neva River,
this “Venice of
the North” is characterized by several islands, many canals and over 600
bridges. It was constructed after a monk
told the future Peter the Great to build a new capital in the north in honor of
St. Peter. He also said that such a city
and its people would be protected by the Kazan Icon, and could not be
conquered. His prediction came true when
the city (then called Leningrad)
withstood 900 days of siege and bombing by the Nazis in World War II, while
incurring horrific casualties and suffering, but did not fall.
The site of the new capital was determined when the Swedish
city of Nienchschants
surrendered in 1703 to Russian forces and a fortress was constructed near the
mouth of the Neva to administer the newly-conquered
territory. After Peter the Great’s historic tours of Europe, St.
Petersburg became the symbol of Russia’s
opening to Western Europe’s
peoples and culture. Thus, it is the site
of the first Russian museum, conservatory, public
library, academy of arts and sciences and university.
It seems appropriate, therefore, that the first meeting of
the European Section of the International Union for the Study of Social Insects
(IUSSI) in Russia (the 3rd European Congress) should meet at the
University of St. Petersburg (22-27
August 2005), which was established in 1724.
To give the meeting further
significance it met in the university’s historic Twelve Collegiums (Ministries)
on Vasilyevski Island, characterized by a series of
twelve identical buildings, connected by a common corridor over 300 yards
long. At one end is a library and at the
other a painting showing Peter the Great signing the university into
existence. The building’s long hallway is
adorned with paintings and sculptures of notable Russian scientists like
Mikhail Lomonosov (first Russian student of the natural sciences),
Andrei Sakharov (father of the soviet H-bomb), and perhaps most celebrated,
Ivan Pavlov, who developed the theory of learning through conditioned response.
Pavlovian learning in insects was in
fact an unstated theme of the Congress.
It was keynoted by Adrian Wenner, Professor
Emeritus, University California
at Santa Barbara
and focused mostly on honey bees, although the other social insects, ants,
wasps and termites also got their due. The
meeting was also considered a collective attempt by social insect scientists in
the rest of Europe to communicate with their colleagues in Russia, who are now beginning to emerge and flourish
as the Soviet Union’s political domination and
persecution of many scientists is on the wane.
How powerful these influences were has been commemorated by a sculpture in
the University’s courtyard dedicated to repression of the faculty during the
Soviet era.
In a metaphoric way, Dr. Wenner
too was ending his self-imposed isolation from bee research as part of this
congress. He was clearly surprised that
so much of his work has been recognized in Russia, when elsewhere it was at
best ignored, at worst denigrated. The
story of Dr. Wenner’s journey from high school
principal and teacher to Professor Emeritus is long and complicated, and can be
found in detail at the World Wide Web site: http://www.beesource.com/pov/wenner/
. He summarized some of it in his discussion,
including rearing queens while working his uncles’ apiary that led him to suspect
that sound would be better ways for bees to communicate the existence of food
sources to their sisters rather than dancing in a dark hive. From this emerged the hypothesis that bees
communicate their own experience via a dance, but do not pass it on to others
through “language.” Thus began a
three-decade struggle first within himself as he saw one of his cherished ideas
(the honey bee’s dance language) shattered, and later with a majority of the
scientific community who were unwilling to reconsider Dr. Karl Von Frisch’s
experiments and conclusions when confronted with conflicting data.
Dr. Wenner provided three
hypotheses at the Congress based on his work:
- “Bees rely on a suite of odors, as well
as on visual and other cues, but do not necessarily rely on a single
chemical at any one time.” This is often
the result of learning (conditioned
response). Bees receiving a
specific odor or suite of odors injected into the colony, for example,
will return to empty dishes to which they were previously trained through
use of these odors.
- “Recruitment
of experienced bees each day can be explained by conditioned response, a recruitment to wherever the odor of similar food
sources exist in the region.” Experienced
forager bees, for example, when receiving a familiar odor, immediately go
to previously yielding sources (learning), while recruits arrive much
later (a learning curve is required).
- “Without
odor, recruited bees cannot find a food source.” Switching to unscented food halfway
through an experiment results in no further recruits. Odorless dishes and paraphernalia are
difficult to make because odor is literally everywhere, and it only takes
a few molecules to train bees. Wind
also complicates foraging experiments as does distance from the hive of a
scent. Finally, bees are easily
distracted by a suite of odors in nature (nectar flow) stronger than those
provided by investigators.
The above, Dr. Wenner concluded,
could be used in practical applications that would encourage bees to forage on
particular crops, something that many have worked on for decades with little
success. This will, however, not be
easy, because it must take into account the “full application of current
knowledge about odor,” including influence of conditioned response (learning),
wind and competing scents found in nature.
Mandyam Srinivasan
and colleagues at the Australian
National University
provided a great deal of information on honey bee learning through some
ingenious experiments that examined honey
bees as they flew through tunnels. These
revealed that bees don’t need a stereo vision mechanism because they estimate
distance based on motion of images.
“Flight speed is regulated by holding constant the average image
velocity as seen by both eyes.” The insects
also hold a constant image velocity with respect to the ground (horizontal
surface), enabling them to gently “touch down” like
airliners as they land.
The authors concluded that research “is beginning to reveal
that these insects may not be the simple, reflexive creatures that they once
were assumed to be.” Bees can learn
general features of flowers and landmarks (color, orientation, symmetry), and
apply them to distinguish between objects not previously encountered. They also exhibit “top-down” processing,
learning to detect poorly visible and camouflaged objects, can navigate through
labyrinths using symbolic signposts, and make associations such as “sameness”
and “difference.”
Finally, in a statement that might have been written by Dr. Wenner himself, the investigators said bees have
“associative recall,” that a familiar scent can trigger recall of an associated
odor, or even of a navigational route to a food location. They concluded, “there
is no hard dichotomy between invertebrates and vertebrates in the context of
perception, learning and ‘cognition,’ and that brain size is not necessarily a
reliable predictor of perceptual capacity.”
A poster by Christoph Grüter
and colleagues, University
of Bern, also provides
evidence of this. They concluded that
“rapid distribution of food amongst hive members via trophallaxis
leads to a fast propagation of olfactory information by means of associated
learning, i.e. a large number of bees has been conditioned to a floral scent during
food processing within the hive.”
Andrew Barron and colleagues, Australian
National University
and University of Illinois,
explored the use of neurohormones, the bioamines serotonin and octopamine,
on dance behavior. Their results when looking
at five different dance components showed a dose-dependent effect using octopamine, but none for serotonin. The conclusion was that this kind of work
will “open a new line of study on this communication system.” Perhaps this will provide additional valuable
information on the various hypotheses surrounding dance language.
Ronald Thenius and colleagues, Karl-Franzens-University,
Austria,
reported that foragers returning with nectar loads hand it off to several
receiving bees. An increase in multiple
uploads does not only affect foragers on a single source, but also the whole
cohort of forager and nectar-receiving bees. This provides evidence that there
are unknown possible mechanisms via these multiple uploads that give the colony
“global information” about its environment.
The symposium in the Congress on “Learning and Memory in
Social Systems” was also overwhelmingly focused on honey bee research. A paper by Uli Müller,
Saarland University, revealed
that molecular machinery (cascades) and learning are interrelated. Another by Dorothea Eisenhardt
and colleagues, Free University of Berlin, showed the basis behind memory
formation was protein synthesis. Bern Grünewald of the same institution reported on cellular
physiology changes in both the antennae and mushroom bodies of the brain due to
conditioning stimulus (learning), and R. Menzel and
P. Szyszka used “optophysiological
recordings” to look at honey bee neurons themselves. Finally Dr. L.V. Rhzhova
and colleagues from the famous Russian Pavlov Institute of Physiology presented
information on glutamate receptors’ role in bee associative learning. Some receptors had a pharmacological profile
similar to that found in mammals.
A related symposium, “The Molecular Bases of Social Behavior
and Sociality,” also prominently featured honey bees. Charles Whitfield and colleagues, University of Illinois
and France’s
INRA, used an “integrated genomic approach to dissect natural behavioral
maturation in the adult worker…” Three
components of gene expression were identified:
- Development
of behavioral competency, complete by eight days.
- Current
behavior state (nurse or forager)
- Experience
They concluded that gene expression was associated with
behavior and not related to age or genotype.
It was also regulated by modulators such as juvenile hormone and not “behavior-consequence
experience or environmental differences.”
Christina Grozinger, North Carolina State University,
studied queen mandibular pheromone’s (QMP) effect on the brain cells
themselves. QMP inhibits worker ovarial development and slows transition for nursing to
foraging. But it [QMP] stimulates
foraging in a bee already determined to be in the forager state. Expression of two genes modulated by QMP (Fz2
and Kr-h1) was
monitored over fours days in young bees, providing further proof that reactions
to pheromones are not “hardwired,” and can be changed (modulated) by the state
of the organism.
Charles Claudianos, Australian National University,
and colleagues reported that honey bees have an unexpected 25-30% deficit in
genes with a total of only 10,000. This
is compared to both the vinegar fly (Drosophila)
that has about 13,000 genes and mosquito, which has close to14,000. They suggested
that the bee has reduced its dependence on genetic diversity by evolving more
specialized nutritional and reproductive strategies as well as complex
cognitive and social behaviors. They also
found a small increase in the number and regulation of key neurological genes
that may account for this shift. The
downside is that “significantly reduced molecular diversity may place the honey
bee at greater risk to environmental change including toxic chemicals.” This does not appear to be good news for
beekeepers who are applying pesticides in colonies for Varroa
control.
In a related paper, Michel Solignac,
CNRS and colleagues from INRA, Saint-Gély-du-fesc , France
provided information that the “linkage map” of the honey bee had been constructed in three steps
with 1694 markers, and is now “saturated” (16 linkage groups). Once a candidate region is defined, the
authors concluded: “…the density of markers increased in the region, and it is
easy to get very close to genes of interest thanks to the relatively small
physical size of the genome (230 megabases).”
Dr. Jozef Šimúth,
Slovak Academy of Sciences and colleagues from Germany
(Berlin and
Halle/Salle) presented their evidence for using the honey bee as a model for
functional genomics. The insect has
several features that make it an ideal organism for the study of both applied
and fundamental functional genomics.
These include: haplo-diploidy, a caste system,
slow ontogeny, rich behavioral potential, and high economic value. Specifically the authors suggested that
several tools could now be developed such as those involved in characterizing
disease resistance genes and stocks, and marker assisted selection for
productivity of royal jelly proteins in
vitro as well as other products used by the pharmaceutical industry. As an example of the above,
K.A. Aronstein, Beneficial Insects Laboratory, Weslaco, Texas
and colleagues have described a novel gene from honey bees (Am18w). They reported that this could be valuable in
understanding how control is regulated in immune related molecules
(anti-microbial peptides) among cells.
Dr. Šimúth presented a plenary lecture
on the potential of using molecular techniques to better understand royal jelly
constituents, which he listed as:
enzymes, nutritional proteins secreted in the larval diet, protective
proteins and peptides put into bee products like royal jelly that protect the
colony against pathogens, and are physiologically active in stimulating the
immune system. All these possibilities
will become more easily explored, he concluded, with release of the honey bee genomic
sequence. The results of the project so
far can bee seen at (http://hgsc.bcm.tmc.edu).
IUSSI Meets in St. Petersburg, Russia: A Feast of Firsts, Part II
By
Dr. Malcolm T.
Sanford
http://apis.shorturl.com
The history of St.
Petersburg is a mixture of cultures, ideas and events
greatly influenced by European ideas after Peter the Great returned from his
legendary travels outside his country.
The list of Russians who contributed to the city’s growth and prosperity
featured in guidebooks for sale in this city is legion. These folks come from all walks of life, and
encompass literature (Dostoevski and Pushkin), dance
(G. Ulanova), music (Tchaikovsky), science (I. Pavlov), and others.
It seemed most suitable that the first meeting of the
European Section of the International Union for the Study of Social Insects
(IUSSI) in Russia
(the 3rd European Congress) would focus some energy on its own
historical roots. This is especially
true since it took place at the University
of St. Petersburg, the
first Russian university established in 1724, and the home of many ideas that
have permeated western science, such as Pavlov’s conditioned learning
response.
The IUSSI was established in 1952. It consists of a number of groups, including
the Russian-speaking section that hosted the St. Petersburg Congress,
and German-speaking and Spanish-speaking (Sección Bolivariana) sections among others. It also publishes the prestigious
journal Insectes Sociaux. There is an international congress every
four years, and the North American section will host the XV such congress in Washington, DC,
July 30 to August 5, 2006. A series of
symposia is already in the works, including:
genes, genomics, social biology and biology; management of social
insects; and a symposium on phylogeny and evolution of bees, dedicated to the
noted biologist Charles Michener, who helped establish the concept of “euosociality,” characterized by reproductive specialization.1
The study of sociality has only comparatively recently been
a focus of insect biologists. Broadly
speaking, there are four groups of “truly social” (eusocial)
insects: ants, bees, wasps and termites.
Study of these has also become more prominent in conjunction with the
rise of “sociobiology,”
a term coined by the famous ant biologist, E.O. Wilson, who will keynote the XV
Congress in Washington, DC. Sociobiology remains a controversial topic,
especially true when the principles found in insects and other organisms are
applied to the human situation.2
In St. Petersburg, Christopher
Starr, University of the West Indes,
and IUSSI’s “archivist,” provided a list of eight
open questions about persons and episodes in sociobiology, which he concluded
“is an immensely rich field that has just begun to be written.” Punctuated with the participants’ popping
caps from Russian beer cans, he explored the following in some detail:
- Relationship
between study in one species, the honey bee, and growth of sociobiology
before 1800.
- Réamur’s book on ants, though complete, remained
unpublished and not mentioned by the author himself. Why not?
- Public
indifference to studies by König and Smeathman on “enormous nests of Macrotermes”(termites) in Africa.
- The 19th
century Dzierzon-led movement to propagate his
scientific approach to honey bee biology.
- The
Marais/Maeterlinck plagiarism dispute concerning articles on Macrotermes.
- Pierre
Paul Grassé’s leadership in the period after
World War II.
- The
emergence of the concept of eusociality by
Charles Michener.
- Concepts
of “kin-group” and “superorganism” are at odds,
but peacefully coexist.
The majority of the above feature honey bees in some way or
another. Both Maeterlinck and Michener
studied bees as well other social insects.
Certainly the accumulated knowledge about honey bees over the centuries
has led to many of the current issues that swirl about insect sociobiology
today. And that field is also expanding
as more and more insects are now being observed through a “social lens.”
As an example, one of
the main presentations at the Russian Congress was by Sumio
Tojo and colleagues, Saga University, Japan
concerning the shield bug, Parastrachia japonenses. This
Japanese “stink bug” (Hemiptera) only feeds on one
plant. Thus, there is little room for
error in its life cycle, and so it has adapted a behavior close to full
sociality, including synchronizing its reproduction with the plant’s,
provisioning food for its young, aggregating during reproductive diapause (cold-weather inactivity), and possessing symbiotic bacteria that supply uric acid in
the gut, which they cannot make themselves.
Sociality in honey bees has been studied a number of ways,
including using sophisticated models. T.
Schmicki and Carlo Crailsheim,
Karl-Franzens-University, Graz reported on their simulation of task
selection in honey bees. In an effort to
add further evidence of sociality reported the literature, which is mostly
subjective (abstract) and based on ant behavior, they created a honey bee
colony simulation that “incorporated stimuli-threshold reinforcement
mechanisms.” This refers to social
insects regulating their division of labor in a decentralized way based on
individual perceptions. Thus, if a
stimulus in the colony exceeds a certain task-specific level or threshold for
an individual, then it moves on to another job.
They used various stimuli including light gradients, brood
pheromone, crop fillings, direct assessment of comb content (brood, pollen, nectar), dances (waggle and trembling) and temporal
(queuing) delays to look at the dynamic between nurse and foraging bees. Their
“simulated” honey bee colony was able to regulate brood nursing, and recruit
nurses, nectar receivers and foragers just like in the real world.
The biogeography of honey bees has never been much of a
focus for investigators in the United
States, perhaps because funding for this
kind of research is extremely scarce. In
addition, the honey bee population in this country is a mixture of introduced
races, impossible to separate into distinct groups or ecotypes. However, bee biodiversity remains of great
interest to Europeans, who can examine native or indigenous honey bee
populations.
Ibrahim Çakmak,
Uludag University, Turkey revealed information on the various races
of Apis mellifera
found in Anatolia. These include A. m. anatoliaca (populations of this
race also differ from east to west inTurkey), A. m. carnica
(Thrace), A. m. armeriaca
(convergence of the Black Sea, Ilgaz and Taurus
mountain ranges), A. m. caucasica (eastern Black Sea), and A. m. syriaca in eastern Turkey,
bordering Syria and Iraq. He concluded
that honey bees in Turkey
differ not only in morphometrics, but also in
foraging behavior, and this diversity can be exploited in finding bees tolerant
to diseases and pests. There is
evidence, however, that hybridization between races is occurring at a rapid
rate due to migratory beekeeping in the country. Fortunately, pockets of endemic bees can
still be found managed by stationary beekeepers.
A.G. Nikolenko, Urfa Scientific Center, Russia,
reported on the critical condition of the black European honey bee (Apis mellifera mellifera). Much of the decline in this subspecies has to
do with habitat destruction; however, hybridization through migratory
beekeeping is also taking a toll. Four
intact local populations, nevertheless, have been identified based on immune
response, measured by levels of antioxidants such as
glyukozo-6-phosphatedehydrogenase.
Immune responses, the author concluded, can be used to separate races
and also be employed in breeding programs.
Specific population buildup data for bees in Northwestern Caucasia, Krasnodar region were also reported by Larisa Moreva,
Kuban State University, Russia.
In a creative departure from collecting experimental data,
Alexander Komissar, National Agricultural University Ukraine, presented a paper based on
anecdotal evidence entitled “Races of Honey Bees, Human Nations and
Religions.” He concluded that the
“distribution of the different aboriginal honey bee races in Europe
coincides with the distribution of main religions.” Thus the Italian (A. m. ligustica) and carniolan
(A. m. carnica)
races are found in Catholic countries, while Macedonian bees (A. m. macedonica)
are associated with countries of the Eastern Orthodox religion. He also said that dark bees (A. m. mellifera)
are not compatible with Catholicism, and protestant countries use exclusively
hybrid bees. Finally, no subspecies is
“naturally distributed” simultaneously in Christian and Moslem countries with
the exception of Albania. From this information, he postulated that the
“introduction of foreign bee races to regions with inappropriate human religion
can’t be successful,” and concluded that “maybe the use of appropriate races
can harmonize human life.” In a
disclaimer sentence, he stated that such a hypothesis is “outside of scientific
understanding,” but so is much of religion, and thus it should not be
discounted.
A symposium on swarming in honey bees somewhat paralleled
that concerning biodiversity. Again Ibrahim Çakmak took the lead by
reporting information on swarming by the various races of honey bees found in Turkey. A. m. caucasica swarms at most once a year in its native
range, producing only ten to twenty queen cells, whereas A. m. carnica may cast as many as three
swarms per season and builds more swarm cells.
Both races, however, swarm far less than A. m. anatoliaca, even though they all
appear to be adapted to cold winters in their native range.
Correlated with higher swarming rates in A. m. anatoliaca
is a dry hot summer season. The same is
true for A. m. syriaca,
which swarms far more frequently and may build hundreds of queen cells. This is due to a combination of several
things, including unpredictable weather (hot, dry desert conditions) and the
fact that it does not have to store as much honey as bees in the north because
the winter season is typically wet enough to promote vegetative flowering. In addition, this race is often challenged by
an enlarged group of predators, including two wasps, known to kill entire
colonies on occasion. Although not
mentioned in the paper, this author would add the bee eating bird (Merops. sp.) to this list. Swarming to avoid predation, therefore, is
not out of the question.
The above observations seem relevant to recent beekeeper
discussions about Russian bees recently introduced into the United States. Users indicate these insects do not produce
as much honey as hybrid bees currently in use, are much more difficult to requeen, and supersede quickly because they continuously
build and tear down many queen cells. Beekeepers
faced with this bee’s behavior, therefore, must adapt their management to its
peculiarities to be successful.
The situation surrounding A. m. syriaca is instructive, given this
author’s recent visit to Iraq
where this bee in endemic. The
traditional basket-like beekeeping in the country employs small colonies of
honey bees and encourages them to swarm.
This kind of management may be much more attuned to the bee’s biological
adaptation than larger Langstroth-type colony
management that is being introduced as a “superior technology.” .
The swarming situation is muddled, Dr. Çakmak
said, “Swarming of bee colonies is known to be affected by a number of factors
in the hive, which include the environment and genetics of honey bee race. However, the collective decision process of
workers to swarm is not well understood.”
Enter the next presentation by Zbiginew
Lipiński, “Emotional Nature of Adaptive Nest
Abandonment by Honeybee Swarms.” Drawing
on observations in other fields of neuroscience, the author explained that
stress rather than reproduction is at the root of all swarming. His “nest abandonment” hypothesis indeed fits
the many scenarios that appear to link swarming (often referred to as
‘reproductive’), migrating and absconding behavior. These may reflect a process,
similar to the nurse-forager model described elsewhere in this article, which finally
leads to release of migratory instinct by highly emotionally excited workers in
shape of a swarm, especially due to insufficiency or lack of control of worker
bee emotions by queen pheromones.
The use of the term “emotional” in the swarming context
raises some eyebrows. A closer
examination of his abstract in the proceedings and presentation at the Congress
suggests that what Dr. Lipiński calls emotional
might be simply “stimulation” or excitation of the central nervous system
(CNS). He concluded that the most common
behavior for all three types of nest abandonment by swarms (adaptive swarming,
migrating and absconding) is stress-related engorgement with honey or
nectar. Expression of other stress
behaviors such as queen cell building, queen rearing, etc. depends, apart from
genetic background of the bee, on type and intensity of a given stress swarm
reaction. Thus, the emotional arousal of
the bee brain and subsequent changes in its cognitive perception not only
affect swarming, migrating and absconding, but other behaviors as well.
References:
- Program
of the XV International Congress on Social Insects, July 30 to August 6,
2006, Washington, DC <http://www.iussi.org/program.html>,
accessed September 25, 2005.
- Wikipedia, Free Internet Encyclopedia <http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Sociobiology>,
accessed September 25, 2005.
-
IUSSI Meets in St. Petersburg, Russia: A Feast of Firsts, Part III
By
Dr. Malcolm T.
Sanford
http://apis.shorturl.com
Every city has a rhythm and St. Petersburg, Russia
is no exception. It is situated at a higher latitude (above the 60th parallel) than
any other great metropolis on the globe.
Summers have very long days; the famous “white nights” in June and early
July are considered a tourist attraction themselves. In addition, the many draw bridges spanning
the Neva River are raised and lowered at specific
times (between 1:30 and 5:00 a.m.) when vehicular traffic is at a minimum. The unaware tourist can easily be
stranded. The population of the city is
devoted to its night life, and the casual visitor’s daily routine also needs to
be adjusted to this reality.
St. Petersburg is two times
zones east of many other great European cities, like Paris,
putting it 8 to 11 hours later than the U.S.’s
Atlantic and Pacific coasts respectively. This means serious jet lag, a great shift in
one’s own daily or circadian rhythm, for many arriving tourists and participants alike attending the 3rd European
Congress on Social Insects.
Insects also have circadian rhythm. Dr. Guy Bloch and colleagues, Alexander Silberman Institute of Life Sciences, Israel, investigated the molecular
basis for this in honey bees. They
reported at the IUSSI Russian congress that “some important features of the
honey bee clock are more similar to mammals than to Drosophila (fruit flies),” and that “behavioral plasticity
(variability) in circadian rhythms is mediated by molecular alternations…” The idea that organisms are regulated in
their natural rhythm at the molecular level may come as a surprise to some, however, there is more and more evidence from various
fields of study that much more happens at that level than previously
recognized. Note the discussions
concerning gene expression, sociality and swarming in previous articles on the
IUSSI congress in this journal.
Regulating temperature can also be a function of the
seasonal rhythm. Thermoregulation was
the theme of one symposium at the St. Petersburg meeting. Honey bee colonies were studied by M. Kleinhenz, Beegroup Würzburg, and colleagues using heat sensitive thermography along with video and endoscopic
recordings in observation hives. They
found that many bees previously thought to be resting by observers were in fact
“brood-heating specialists.” These
workers placed their bodies in contact with the comb and had specific antennal
and abdominal actions different than those found in resting bees. A definite “hot spot” could be seen when a
brood-heating specialist bee that had previously been in contact with comb was
removed from a specific location. In
addition, the authors reported that heating specialists ensconced themselves in
empty cells and could in this manner heat up to six adjacent cells containing
brood. Similar behavior has been
observed in paper wasps.
Julia Jones, University
of Sydney, and colleagues
described how learning and memory are affected by rearing temperatures. They reported that individuals with different
patrilines (drone fathers) vary in thermoregulation,
leading to the conclusion that “genetically diverse colonies are better able to
maintain a stable brood temperature than genetically uniform colonies.” In addition, it appears that short-term
memory is affected by temperature variation during pupal
development, whereas long-term is not.
Alexander Komissar, National Agricultural University Ukraine, provided a description
of his experiments wintering baby nuclei that had top-mounted
electric heaters. These provided a small amount of heat that then
developed into a "vertical temperature gradient" in the hives.
Bees distributed themselves throughout the gradient from 15-36 degrees C. (59 –
96.8 F), in specific ways with most active movement above 25 degrees C (77
F). Queens preferred the zone above 26
degrees C (78.8 F). The heater-equipped nuclei over wintered successfully
and two professional Ukrainian beekeepers use this method for mass
storage of two-frame nucs in a temperate climate
similar to northern USA or Canada (five months without a cleansing
flight) with the aim of selling them in the spring. .
The tropics presents other
challenges in terms of temperature extremes.
Through an elaborate series of experiments, Yaacov
Lensky, Emeritus Professor Triwaks
Bee Laboratory, Rehovot,
Israel reported
on behavior of individual workers and colonies at high summer and low winter
temperatures. Individual workers were
resistant to as much as 50 degrees C (122 F), and continued foraging up to 48
degrees C (118.4 F). Evaporative cooling
can reduce hive temperature, but it was concluded that keeping bees in white
colonies was perhaps the best practical strategy. In addition, protecting colonies from winter
cold either through use of black paint or Infrared-Polyethylene (PE)-covered
enclosures was found to be effective.
From these experiments, Dr. Lensky concluded:
“Protection of bee colonies from extreme temperatures enhanced worker
population size and production of spring and late summer honey yield, even in a
mild Mediterranean-type subtropical climate.”
As would be expected, diseases and parasites got a good deal
of play during the congress. There was a
general session on these concerning both bumble bees and honey bees, with a
special symposium on the latter insects, entitled: “Can European Honeybees
Coexist With Varroa Mites?”
Mark Brown, Trinity
College, Dublin, Ireland,
reviewed current knowledge of host-parasite relations in a plenary or keynote
lecture, which he said remains unclear in many areas. He concluded that high levels of redundancy
in social insect colonies and patterns of division of labor act to reduce the
apparent ecological cost of parasitism.
A variety of examples of parasites were reported at the congress,
including so-called “social parasites” that take advantage of interactions between members of a social host
species. There is a bewildering array of
these kinds of organisms and they run the gamut from being extremely to
slightly detrimental. In addition, there
are “inquilines” found in social insect nests that are considered benign, even
beneficial. One hypothesis is that any
parasite over time is embarked on a slow road to becoming (evolving) less
damaging to its host, finally reaching a kind of equilibrium (mutualism) as an inquiline, where both host and now ex-parasite come to
depend on each other for survival.
A symposium on
social insects and their macroparasites provided some
examples of social parasitism that while appearing to be unique have relevance to other social insect situations. A. Lenoir and colleagues University
of Tours, France studied two kinds of ant
queens, small (microgynes) and large (macrogynes), found in the same species (Manica rubida).
Two hypotheses are put forth to explain why they differ: 1) small queens are social parasites involved
in nest takeovers or 2) small queens establish nests in the region of the
mother nest, whereas larger individuals fly long distances, providing two
dispersal strategies. Carlos Lopez-Vaamonde and colleagues Zoological Society of London looked
at bumble bee (Bombus terrestris)
workers that invade unrelated colonies and produce male offspring in an effort
to control a colony’s genetics. The same
is true in slave-making ants studied by E. Brunner and colleagues, University
Regensburg, Germany, who reported unrelated worker ants producing up to 100% of
males in nests. Related to this is
social parasitism found in honey bees when workers of Apis mellifera capensis
invade colonies of Apis mellifera scutellata in Africa. Instead of producing males, however, invading
workers (false queens) are able to produce workers, which then take over the
nest. This has become a series problem
in certain areas when scutellata
colonies were moved into capensis
territory or vice versa.
M. X.
Ruiz-Gonzalez and colleagues, Trinity College, Dublin, Ireland tested the haploid
susceptibility hypothesis. Males from
unfertilized eggs (haploid) are thought to be more susceptible to diseases
since they don’t have another set of alleles (genes) found in diploid
individuals, which might confer resistance by masking the effects of a single
damaging allele (gene). Infecting bumble
bee (Bombus terrestris)
colonies with a parasite (Crithidia bombi), however, revealed no difference in
susceptibility between diploid workers and haploid males. That looks to be good news for those
interested in bumble bee welfare in Europe who
are concerned about social parasitism through
production of males.
Another pathogen,
Nosema bombi, is
also being studied in Bombus terrestris,
which can devastate bumble bee colonies used in greenhouse pollination. I. Fries and colleagues, Swedish University
of Agricultural Science, Uppsala
reported great variability in this organism in different bumble bee hosts. The rate of infection is also quite variable
ranging from 1-3% in Sweden
and Switzerland to about 13%
elsewhere (Denmark, the Netherlands), according to Julia Klee, Queen’s
University, Belfast,
Ireland. Infection is horizontal (larva to larva), not
vertical (egg to larva), according to J. van der
Steen, Wageningen
University, the Netherlands. Finally, O. Otti
and colleagues ETH-Zurich, Switzerland tested the effects of
infestation, finding infected colonies generally smaller and producing few if
any sexual forms. They concluded that
severe effects from Nosema bombi
mean that it’s opportunity to affect the next host generation is minimal. Some of these findings may also apply to the
relationship between Nosema apis,
and its honey bee host.
Exotic organisms
also affect social insects in many ways.
Best known are those that have relatively recently been introduced to
managed honey bee colonies, such as the small hive beetle (Aethina tumida) and the Asian mite, Varroa destructor. This author presented the most recent
information on small hive beetle.
Introduced in 1998 from Africa, this insect has greatly changed honey
house management in the U.S. Its introduction shows in a very real way how
transportation of organisms around the globe can affect great change in local
ecosystems.
Of course nothing
shows the effects of exotic organisms more than introduction of Varroa to Apis mellifera in the 1950s when, this mite transferred from
its original host Apis cerana or indica. This has literally transformed apiculture on
a worldwide basis and continues doing so to this day. In a symposium on whether Varroa
can coexist with European bees, Adrian Wenner, University of California,
Santa Barbara
answered in the affirmative. His studies
on populations isolated on offshore islands and in wilderness preserves show that wild or feral
colonies (not treated by beekeepers) have survived Varroa
depredations for a number of years, especially those with mixed genetics. The makes for what he calls the “exciting
potential” of “survivor” colonies that
are being increasingly studied around the world, including Dr. John Kefuss and colleagues who have incorporated Apis mellifera intermissa (the
so-called “tell” bee in Tunisia) and the “Elgon” stock, a mixture of Buckfast and Apis mellifera monticola (mountain
African), developed in Sweden.
Ingemar Fries,
University of Agricultural Sciences, Upsalla reported
on Swedish experiments, which amount to little more than isolating infested
colonies on an island and monitoring survival and various other mite
infestation parameters. Starting with
150 colonies, the results are that five original and five daughter colonies
(swarms) are surviving six years later.
During this interval there has been large variability across the
spectrum of measurements. Thus, winter
survival and swarming tendency both have increased and decreased over the
period and mite infestation rate of colonies without brood decreased the fifth
year of infestation. The experiment continues
with the conclusion
that “some sort of parasite adaptation has occurred, ensuring the survival
of both host and parasite.”
P. Rosenkranz, University of Hohenheim,
Stuttgart, Germany also reported on
experiments looking at Varroa tolerance. Carniolan bees (Apis mellifera carnica) from Hohenheim (H)
were compared with “survivor” colonies on the island of Gotland
(G), left untreated since 1999 even after a dramatic mortality in 2002. In the year 2004, average initial
infestations of 700 mites (G) and 400 (H) were calculated. The bee populations were 6,000 adult bees and
10,500 (G) and 14,000 (H) brood cells respectively. At the end of the season, G colonies had 16,500 while H
decreased to 10,500 bees. Highest
absolute infestations in August were 9,000 mites (H) and 6,500 (G). By the end of the year, all the H colonies
had perished showing typical symptoms of heavy Varroa
infestation. Only one G colony was lost
due to queen problems, but the rest subsequently died during winter ending the
experiment. Nevertheless, it is
concluded that in spite of
higher start infestation and higher brood mite counts throughout
the season, it was clear that G colonies had significantly lower infestation
rates at the end of the season. Thus, “preliminary results indicate that the
G colonies have established mechanisms to reduce the increase of the Varroa population.”
This author
provided information on the current status of “soft chemicals” in controlling Varroa in the U.S.
and Canada. Topping the list are essential oils (tymol) and organic acids (formic, oxalic, lactic), the use
of which was pioneered in Europe. There is little question that chemical
treatment by beekeepers has kept Varroa in check, but
the effective hard chemicals, fluvalinate (Apistan®), coumpahos (CheckMite+®) and amitraz, are now losing effectiveness as the mites continue
to become resistant. There is less
likelihood of this happening when beekeepers use hard pesticides in conjunction
with soft chemicals and what are termed “biotechnical” controls (drone
trapping, screened bottom boards). All
this together is called integrated control or integrated pest management (IPM).
Specific and
effective integrated control has been developed in the Netherlands. J.M. van der steen, Wageningen University, closed out the symposium by
describing two regimens published in a standard Varroa
control calendar, which do not incorporate hard chemicals. In the “default” situation (light mite
infestation), drone trapping is recommended from April through June, followed
by formic acid or thymol (Thymovar®)
July through September. In the
“emergency” scenario (heavy mite infestation), formic or lactic acid is
recommended January and February, formic or thymol or
drone trapping April and May (no treatment in March), followed by formic or thymol from June to September. Finally, October through December, the
beekeeper returns to the January through February regimen.
Although not
strictly a honey bee meeting, the 3rd European Congress on Social
Insects incorporated many studies on the biology of Apis mellifera and other social insects, the
results of which could be of importance to both scientists and beekeepers. It is in this vein, that I have seen fit to
report and the editor to print some of the highlights. This provides a small window into the study
of social insects, perhaps encouraging some readers to attend 15th
International Congress of the International Union for the Study of Social Insects
in Washington, DC, July 30 through August 5, 2006 <http://www.iussi.org/iussi2006.html>.